
What does this have to do with memory? The cleverest part of this research is that after training infants on one of these tasks for a couple of days, Rovee-Collier later tested whether they remembered it. Infants again learn the game quickly, and press on the lever significantly more when it makes the train move. Now every time the infant presses on it, the train will move around its track. At first, the lever doesn’t work, and the experimenters measure how much a baby naturally presses down. But instead of lying in a crib – which this age group just won’t do for very long – the infant sits on their parent’s lap with their hands on a lever that will eventually make a train move around a track. The version for 6- to 18-month-old infants is similar. As you might imagine, infants quickly learn that they’re in control – they like seeing the mobile move and so they kick more than before the string was attached to their leg, showing they’ve learned that kicking makes the mobile move. Next, they tie a string from the baby’s leg to the end of the mobile, so that whenever the baby kicks, the mobile moves. They measure how much the baby kicks to get an idea of their natural propensity to move their legs.

In the version for 2- to 6-month-old infants, researchers place an infant in a crib with a mobile hanging overhead. So the key to Rovee-Collier’s research was devising a task that was sensitive to babies’ rapidly changing bodies and abilities in order to assess their memories over a long period. Of course, infants can’t exactly tell you what they remember.

Research from psychologist Carolyn Rovee-Collier’s lab in the 1980s and 1990s famously showed that infants can form some of these other kinds of memories from an early age.
#Infantile amnesia how to
There are also procedural memories, or memories for how to perform an action, like opening your front door or driving a car. There are semantic memories, or memories of facts, like the names for different varieties of apples, or the capital of your home state. In fact, there are lots of different kinds of memories besides those that are autobiographical.

A few months later, infants can demonstrate that they remember lots of familiar faces by smiling most at the ones they see most often. Within the first few days of life, infants can recall their own mother’s face and distinguish it from the face of a stranger.

Infants can form memoriesĭespite the fact that people can’t remember much before the age of 2 or 3, research suggests that infants can form memories – just not the kinds of memories you tell about yourself. Here’s what researchers know about babies and memory. But why can’t we remember the things that happened to us when we were infants? Does memory start to work only at a certain age? In fact, most people can’t remember events from the first few years of their lives – a phenomenon researchers have dubbed infantile amnesia. Some students talk about their first day of pre-K others talk about a time when they got hurt or upset some cite the day their younger sibling was born.ĭespite vast differences in the details, these memories do have a couple of things in common: They’re all autobiographical, or memories of significant experiences in a person’s life, and they typically didn’t happen before the age of 2 or 3. Whenever I teach about memory in my child development class at Rutgers University, I open by asking my students to recall their very first memories. The following essay is reprinted with permission from The Conversation, an online publication covering the latest research.
